open access

Vol 62, No 4 (2010)
Original article
Submitted: 2013-02-18
Published online: 2011-02-24
Get Citation

Perceived learning outcome: The relationship between experience, realism and situation awareness during simulator training

Evelyn-Rose Saus, BjØrn Helge Johnsen, Jarle EidEvelyn-Rose Saus, BjØrn Helge Johnsen, Jarle Eid
IMH 2010;62(4):258-264.

open access

Vol 62, No 4 (2010)
MARITIME PSYCHOLOGY Original article
Submitted: 2013-02-18
Published online: 2011-02-24

Abstract


Background. Navigation errors are a frequent cause of serious accidents and work-related injuries among seafarers. The present study investigated the effects of experience, perceived realism, and situation awareness (SA) on the perceived learning outcome of simulator-based navigation training.
Material and methods. Thirty-two Norwegian Navy officer cadets were assigned to a low and a high mental workload conditions based on previous educational and navigational experience.
Results. In the low mental workload condition, experience (negatively associated), perceived realism, and subjective SA explained almost half of the total variance in perceived learning outcome. A hierarchical regression analysis showed that only subjective SA made a unique contribution to the learning outcome. In the high mental workload condition, perceived realism and subjective SA together explained almost half of the variance in perceived learning outcome. Furthermore, both perceived realism and subjective SA were shown to make an independent contribution to perceived learning outcomes.
Conclusions. The results of this study show that in order to enhance the learning outcomes from simulator training it is necessary to design training procedures and scenarios that enable students to achieve functional fidelity and to generate and maintain SA during training. This can further improve safety and reduce the risk of maritime disasters. (Int Marit Health 2010; 61; 4: 258-264)

Abstract


Background. Navigation errors are a frequent cause of serious accidents and work-related injuries among seafarers. The present study investigated the effects of experience, perceived realism, and situation awareness (SA) on the perceived learning outcome of simulator-based navigation training.
Material and methods. Thirty-two Norwegian Navy officer cadets were assigned to a low and a high mental workload conditions based on previous educational and navigational experience.
Results. In the low mental workload condition, experience (negatively associated), perceived realism, and subjective SA explained almost half of the total variance in perceived learning outcome. A hierarchical regression analysis showed that only subjective SA made a unique contribution to the learning outcome. In the high mental workload condition, perceived realism and subjective SA together explained almost half of the variance in perceived learning outcome. Furthermore, both perceived realism and subjective SA were shown to make an independent contribution to perceived learning outcomes.
Conclusions. The results of this study show that in order to enhance the learning outcomes from simulator training it is necessary to design training procedures and scenarios that enable students to achieve functional fidelity and to generate and maintain SA during training. This can further improve safety and reduce the risk of maritime disasters. (Int Marit Health 2010; 61; 4: 258-264)
Get Citation

Keywords

novice; expert; navigation; military

About this article
Title

Perceived learning outcome: The relationship between experience, realism and situation awareness during simulator training

Journal

International Maritime Health

Issue

Vol 62, No 4 (2010)

Article type

Original article

Pages

258-264

Published online

2011-02-24

Page views

1639

Article views/downloads

2009

Bibliographic record

IMH 2010;62(4):258-264.

Keywords

novice
expert
navigation
military

Authors

Evelyn-Rose Saus
BjØrn Helge Johnsen
Jarle EidEvelyn-Rose Saus
BjØrn Helge Johnsen
Jarle Eid

References (21)
  1. Hyun K. A Primary Prevention Training Model for the Unmet Needs of Newly Arrived Korean Immigrants. Prevention in Human Services. 2008; 12(1): 25–41.
  2. Eraut M. Non-formal learning and tacit knowledge in professional work. Br J Educ Psychol. 2000; 70 ( Pt 1): 113–136.
  3. Doane SM, Sohn YW, Jodlowski MT. Pilot ability to anticipate the consequences of flight actions as a function of expertise. Hum Factors. 2004; 46(1): 92–103.
  4. Hetherington C, Flin R, Mearns K. Safety in shipping: the human element. J Safety Res. 2006; 37(4): 401–411.
  5. Saus ER, Johnsen B, Eid J, et al. The Effect of brief situational awareness training in a police shooting simulator: An experimental study. Military Psychology. 2006; 18(Suppl): S3–S21.
  6. Saus ER. Johnsen BH, Eid J, Thayer JF. Who benefits from simulator training: Personality and its effects on situational awareness in navigation training. Submitted. 2010.
  7. Dahlstrom N, Nahlinder S. Mental Workload in Aircraft and Simulator During Basic Civil Aviation Training. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology. 2009; 19(4): 309–325.
  8. Brannick M, Prince C, Salas E. Can PC-Based Systems Enhance Teamwork in the Cockpit? The International Journal of Aviation Psychology. 2005; 15(2): 173–187.
  9. Fothergill S, Loft S, Neal A. ATC-lab(Advanced): an air traffic control simulator with realism and control. Behav Res Methods. 2009; 41(1): 118–127.
  10. Prince C, Salas E. Situation assessment for routine flight and decision making. Int J Cogn Ergon. 1998; 1: 315–324.
  11. Carretta TR, Ree MJ. Determinants of SA in U.S. Air Force F-15 Pilots. Paper presented at the Aerospace Medical Panel Symposium held in Brussels, Belgium 24-27 April 1995 Brussels, 1996.
  12. Prince C, Ellis E, Brannick M, et al. Measurement of Team Situation Awareness in Low Experience Level Aviators. International Journal of Aviation Psychology. 2007; 17(1): 41–57.
  13. Waag WL, Houck MR. Tools for assessing situational awareness in an operational fighter environment. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1994; 65(5 Suppl): A13–A19.
  14. Klaver CH, de Gues EJC, Vries J. Ambulatory monitoring system. In: Maarse FJ, Akkerman AR, Brand AN, Mulder LJM, Van der Stelt MJ. ed. Computers in psychology: Applications, methods and instrumentation. The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse 1994: 254–268.
  15. Mulder LJM, de Waard D, Brookhuis KA. Estimating mental effort using heart rate and heart rate variability. In: Stanton N, Hedge A, Brookhuis K, Salas E, Hendrick H. ed. Handbook of human factors and ergonomics methods. Washington D.C . Taylor & Francis, Washington 2004: 201–208.
  16. Mulder LJ. Measurement and analysis methods of heart rate and respiration for use in applied environments. Biol Psychol. 1992; 34(2-3): 205–236.
  17. Endsley MR. The role of situation awareness in naturalistic decision making. In: Zsambok CE, Klein G. ed. Naturalistic decision making. . New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Publishers, Mahwah 1997: 269–283.
  18. Underwood G, Chapman P, Bowden K, et al. Visual search while driving: skill and awareness during inspection of the scene. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. 2002; 5(2): 87–97.
  19. O'Brien KS, O'Hare D. Situational awareness ability and cognitive skills training in a complex real-world task. Ergonomics. 2007; 50(7): 1064–1091.
  20. Issenberg SB, McGaghie WC, Petrusa ER, et al. Features and uses of high-fidelity medical simulations that lead to effective learning: a BEME systematic review. Med Teach. 2005; 27(1): 10–28.
  21. Weller JM. Simulation in undergraduate medical education: bridging the gap between theory and practice. Med Educ. 2004; 38(1): 32–38.

Regulations

Important: This website uses cookies. More >>

The cookies allow us to identify your computer and find out details about your last visit. They remembering whether you've visited the site before, so that you remain logged in - or to help us work out how many new website visitors we get each month. Most internet browsers accept cookies automatically, but you can change the settings of your browser to erase cookies or prevent automatic acceptance if you prefer.

By VM Media Group sp. z o.o., Grupa Via Medica, ul. Świętokrzyska 73, 80–180 Gdańsk, Poland

tel.: +48 58 320 94 94, fax:+48 58 320 94 60, e-mail: viamedica@viamedica.pl