open access

Vol 73, No 3 (2022)
Original article
Submitted: 2022-09-29
Accepted: 2022-09-30
Published online: 2022-10-10
Get Citation

Impact of work exposure on cognitive performance in Faroese deep-sea fishers: a field study

Annbjørg Selma Abrahamsen12, Pál Weihe12, Wessel M.A. van Leeuwen3, Fróði Debes1
·
Pubmed: 36217969
·
IMH 2022;73(3):150-161.
Affiliations
  1. Department of Occupational Medicine and Public Health, The Faroese Hospital System, Tórshavn, Faroe Islands
  2. Faculty of Health Sciences, The Faroese University, Torshavn, Faroe Islands
  3. Department of Psychology, Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden

open access

Vol 73, No 3 (2022)
MARITIME PSYCHOLOGY Original article
Submitted: 2022-09-29
Accepted: 2022-09-30
Published online: 2022-10-10

Abstract

Background: This study examines the impact of work-related exposure on the cognitive performance of Faroese deep-sea fishers. Faroese fishing crews work long hours in demanding and noisy environments amidst highly uncertain and challenging weather conditions. These factors, together with compromised patterns of rest and sleep, are known to increase fatigue. Our aim was to study if changes could be measured in fishers’ cognitive performance at the end of the trip when compared with the baseline measure at the beginning.

Materials and methods: Data was collected over 15 months (May 2017 to July 2018) from 157 fishers on 18 fishing trips which involved 202 investigative days on board. Questionnaires and six computerised cognitive tests: Simple Reaction Time, Numeric Working Memory, Corsi Blocks, Rapid Visual Information Processing, Digit Vigilance, and Card Sorting Test were used for data collection at the beginning and end of the trip. Differences between the outcomes on the two test points were analysed with one-way ANOVA comparing the performances at the beginning and end of the voyage, and two-way ANOVA to examine the interactive effect of chronotype and test occasions on the outcomes. Mixed models were used to test for the effects of predictor variables.

Results: Significant declines in cognitive performance were observed from the beginning to the end of the trip, with decreases in visuospatial memory and reaction times, and increases in cognitive lapses. Furthermore, slowing in response times was observed in the second half of the Digit Vigilance test when comparing the halves.

Conclusions: Declines in performance were observed from the start to the end of the trip. Furthermore, fishers performed significantly worse in the second half of some parted tests, and evening types seem less influenced by irregular work hours. These findings call for improving the safety of the vessels and their crew.

Abstract

Background: This study examines the impact of work-related exposure on the cognitive performance of Faroese deep-sea fishers. Faroese fishing crews work long hours in demanding and noisy environments amidst highly uncertain and challenging weather conditions. These factors, together with compromised patterns of rest and sleep, are known to increase fatigue. Our aim was to study if changes could be measured in fishers’ cognitive performance at the end of the trip when compared with the baseline measure at the beginning.

Materials and methods: Data was collected over 15 months (May 2017 to July 2018) from 157 fishers on 18 fishing trips which involved 202 investigative days on board. Questionnaires and six computerised cognitive tests: Simple Reaction Time, Numeric Working Memory, Corsi Blocks, Rapid Visual Information Processing, Digit Vigilance, and Card Sorting Test were used for data collection at the beginning and end of the trip. Differences between the outcomes on the two test points were analysed with one-way ANOVA comparing the performances at the beginning and end of the voyage, and two-way ANOVA to examine the interactive effect of chronotype and test occasions on the outcomes. Mixed models were used to test for the effects of predictor variables.

Results: Significant declines in cognitive performance were observed from the beginning to the end of the trip, with decreases in visuospatial memory and reaction times, and increases in cognitive lapses. Furthermore, slowing in response times was observed in the second half of the Digit Vigilance test when comparing the halves.

Conclusions: Declines in performance were observed from the start to the end of the trip. Furthermore, fishers performed significantly worse in the second half of some parted tests, and evening types seem less influenced by irregular work hours. These findings call for improving the safety of the vessels and their crew.

Get Citation

Keywords

fishers, fatigue, cognitive decline, Digit Vigilance, Simple Reaction Time, Visuospatial Memory, the Psychomotor Vigilance Tests

About this article
Title

Impact of work exposure on cognitive performance in Faroese deep-sea fishers: a field study

Journal

International Maritime Health

Issue

Vol 73, No 3 (2022)

Article type

Original article

Pages

150-161

Published online

2022-10-10

Page views

3977

Article views/downloads

301

DOI

10.5603/IMH.2022.0028

Pubmed

36217969

Bibliographic record

IMH 2022;73(3):150-161.

Keywords

fishers
fatigue
cognitive decline
Digit Vigilance
Simple Reaction Time
Visuospatial Memory
the Psychomotor Vigilance Tests

Authors

Annbjørg Selma Abrahamsen
Pál Weihe
Wessel M.A. van Leeuwen
Fróði Debes

References (38)
  1. Faroese, Government. Hagstovan. https://hagstova.fo/fo/b%25C3%25BAskapur/uttanlandshandil/%25 C3%25BAtflutningur-av-v%25C3%25B8rum.
  2. Jepsen JR, Zhao Z, van Leeuwen WMA. Seafarer fatigue: a review of risk factors, consequences for seafarers' health and safety and options for mitigation. Int Marit Health. 2015; 66(2): 106–117.
  3. International Maritime Organisation. Fatigue factors in manning and safety. In: Fatigue Factors in Manning and Safety. Vol. A. 772; 1993: 7. https://wwwcdn.imo.org/localresources/en/KnowledgeCentre/IndexofIMOResolutions/AssemblyDocuments/A.772(18).pdf.
  4. Hansen JH, Holmen IM. Sleep disturbances among offshore fleet workers: a questionnaire-based survey. Int Marit Health. 2011; 62(2): 123–130.
  5. Breidahl T, Christensen M, Jepsen JR, et al. The influence of ship movements on the energy expenditure of fishermen. A study during a North Sea voyage in calm weather. Int Marit Health. 2013; 64(3): 114–120.
  6. Zhou Y, Daamen W, Vellinga T, et al. Impacts of wind and current on ship behavior in ports and waterways: A quantitative analysis based on AIS data. Ocean Eng. 2020; 213: 107774.
  7. Christiansen JM, Hovmand SR. Prevention of Accidents at Work in Nordic Fisheries – What Has Worked. 2017.
  8. Roberts S, Williams J. Update of Mortality for Workers in the UK Merchant Shipping and Fishing Sectors. Vol 1976; 2005.
  9. Raby M, Lee JD. Fatigue and Workload in the maritime industry. In: Stress, Workload, and Fatigue. In P.A. Ha. Mahwah. Lawrence Erlbaum Sssociates., New Jersey 2000: 566–578.
  10. Strauch B. Investigating fatigue in marine accident investigations. Procedia Manufacturing. 2015; 3: 3115–3122.
  11. Knauth P. Extended work periods. Ind Health. 2007; 45(1): 125–136.
  12. Wadsworth EJK, Allen PH, McNamara RL, et al. Fatigue and health in a seafaring population. Occup Med (Lond). 2008; 58(3): 198–204.
  13. Jensen OCC, Petursdottir G, Holmen IM, et al. A review of fatal accident incidence rate trends in fishing. Int Marit Health. 2014; 65(2): 47–52.
  14. A Høvdanum AS, Jensen OC, Petursdóttir G, et al. A review of fatigue in fishermen: a complicated and underprioritised area of research. Int Marit Health. 2014; 65(3): 166–172.
  15. Akerstedt T, Knutsson A, Westerholm P, et al. Sleep disturbances, work stress and work hours: a cross-sectional study. J Psychosom Res. 2002; 53(3): 741–748.
  16. Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, et al. The multidimensional fatigue inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1995; 39(3): 315–325.
  17. Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGerald P, et al. The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br J Clin Psychol. 1982; 21(1): 1–16.
  18. Dorrian J, NL R, Dinges D, Kushida CA. Psychomotor vigilance performance: Neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleeploss; pp. 39-70. In: Kushida CA, ed. Sleep Deprivation Clinical Issues, Pharmachology, and Sleep Loss Effects. Vol. 193. 1st ed. Marcel Dekker, New York 2005: 39–70.
  19. Horne JA, Wilkinson S. Chronic sleep reduction: daytime vigilance performance and EEG measures of sleepiness, with particular reference to "practice" effects. Psychophysiology. 1985; 22(1): 69–78.
  20. Tao M, Yang D, Liu W. Learning effect and its prediction for cognitive tests used in studies on indoor environmental quality. Energy Build. 2019; 197: 87–98.
  21. Lee M, Kleitman N. Studies on the physiology of sleep. II. Attempts to demonstrate functional changes in the nervous system during experimental insomnia. Am J Physiol-Legacy Content. 1923; 67(1): 141–152.
  22. Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000; 6(3): 236–249.
  23. Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Mullington JM, et al. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003; 26(2): 117–126.
  24. Hansen JH. Shift work in the offshore vessel fleet: circadian rhythms and cognitive performance. Dr thesis. 2012;39. https://brage.bibsys.no/xmlui//handle/11250/244989.
  25. Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997; 20(4): 267–277.
  26. Tilley AJ, Wilkinson RT. The effects of a restricted sleep regime on the composition of sleep and on performance. Psychophysiology. 1984; 21(4): 406–412.
  27. Barnett M, Zhao Z, van Leeuwen WMA. Project MARTHA: The Final Report. 2017(January).
  28. Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011; 34(5): 581–591.
  29. van Leeuwen WMA, Kircher A, Dahlgren A, et al. Sleep, sleepiness, and neurobehavioral performance while on watch in a simulated 4 hours on/8 hours off maritime watch system. Chronobiol Int. 2013; 30(9): 1108–1115.
  30. Basner M, Mollicone D, Dinges D. Validity and sensitivity of a brief psychomotor vigilance test (PVT-B) to total and partial sleep deprivation. Acta Astronautica. 2011; 69(11-12): 949–959.
  31. Doran SM, Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol. 2001; 139(3): 253–267.
  32. Smith MG, Wusk GC, Nasrini J, et al. Effects of six weeks of chronic sleep restriction with weekend recovery on cognitive performance and wellbeing in high-performing adults. Sleep. 2021; 44(8).
  33. Olafsdóttir L. The relationship between fishermen's health and sleeping habits. Work. 2004; 22(1): 57–61.
  34. Ólafsdóttir L. Áhrif hvíldar á heilsu og öryggi sjómanna Rannsókn gerð á íslenskum sjómönnum : Á vegum Samgönguráðuneytisins Maí 2004 Solarplexus ehf. 2004: 1–97.
  35. Web W. Partial and Differential Sleep Deprivation. In: Kales A, Lippincott J, eds. Sleep, Physiology and Pathology: A Synopsium. JB Lippincott, Philadelphia 1969: 221–231.
  36. Abrahamsen A, Weihe P, Fróði D, et al. Sleep, sleepiness, and fatigue on board faroese fishing vessels. 2022(December 2021): 347–362.
  37. Deutsch J. The effects of time of day on cognitive performance in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011; 7(3): 3–7.
  38. van Leeuwen WMA, Pekcan C, Barnett M, et al. Mathematical modelling of sleep and sleepiness under various watch keeping schedules in the maritime industry. Marine Policy. 2021; 130: 104277.

Regulations

Important: This website uses cookies. More >>

The cookies allow us to identify your computer and find out details about your last visit. They remembering whether you've visited the site before, so that you remain logged in - or to help us work out how many new website visitors we get each month. Most internet browsers accept cookies automatically, but you can change the settings of your browser to erase cookies or prevent automatic acceptance if you prefer.

By VM Media Group sp. z o.o., Grupa Via Medica, ul. Świętokrzyska 73, 80–180 Gdańsk, Poland

tel.: +48 58 320 94 94, fax:+48 58 320 94 60, e-mail: viamedica@viamedica.pl